14

Good readers have distinct brain anatomy, research reveals

Here is a link to the academic article:

https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/39550055/

What I find cool, is that the study involved an open source data set with brain imaging data of a 1000 individuals (the Human Connectome Project Young Adult dataset). Anyone could have downloaded the data set, done the analysis, and scored a publication in Nueroimage. (assuming, you are smart, and have the drive of course..)

19 hours agoisaacfrond

Anyone has advice for reading better and faster? I always seem to get distracted midway through sentences. By the time I get to the end I forgot what was there at the beginning.

18 hours agoksynwa

When I was younger (and reading things I wasn't interested in) I noticed I would often read a sentence but not absorb it, proceeding to the next regardless. I saw the words, I just wasn't actively focused on their meaning, and kept going. It became a reflex to just keep going, until the end of the paragraph and I had missed a lot.

I corrected it by not allowing myself to proceed until I had re-read the sentence and absorbed the meaning. It sometimes would take multiple shots, and it slowed my reading considerably. Eventually though, I lost the habit. Fwiw.

17 hours agokylecazar

Read more printed books, away from distractions. There's also an element of slow being fast. Trying to read faster often reduces your comprehension.

15 hours agomarginalia_nu

Stop quick context switching (social media, phone notifications, etc) and just read*.

That's it.

Your brain will eventually adapt to not jumping all over the place every 30 seconds. But you must train it to reach that point, it won't happen over night.

[*] preferably good literature*

17 hours agoGeoAtreides

Easy test for good literature:

- if you get in bed with a book and fall asleep in 10 min, it's bad

- if you get in bed with a book and oh shit it's 4 am and you've finished it, it's good

Doesn't apply to books you expect to learn something from, but it does to fiction.

15 hours agonottorp

I think those are two different things: a) absorbing what you're reading and b) speed.

Re absorbing, I suspect that's more about your current mindset, are you tired, etc.

Re speed, one trick that I've found works is to force yourself to read faster by using a finger/something under the words, which moves at a faster pace than comfortable, and you just try to keep up with it. It's a bit uncomfortable, but after awhile you will find you are reading faster. This is similar to the strategy used by people learning to memorize things more quickly described in https://archive.nytimes.com/www.nytimes.com/interactive/2011...

"No matter how much I practiced, I couldn’t memorize playing cards any faster than 1 every 10 seconds. I was stuck in a rut, and I couldn’t figure out why. “My card times have hit a plateau,” I lamented.

MemoryTerry “I would recommend you check out the literature on speed typing,” he replied.

When people first learn to use a keyboard, they improve very quickly from sloppy single-finger pecking to careful two-handed typing, until eventually the fingers move effortlessly and the whole process becomes unconscious. At this point, most people’s typing skills stop progressing. They reach a plateau. If you think about it, it’s strange. We’ve always been told that practice makes perfect, and yet many people sit behind a keyboard for hours a day. So why don’t they just keeping getting better and better?

In the 1960s, the psychologists Paul Fitts and Michael Posner tried to answer this question by describing the three stages of acquiring a new skill. During the first phase, known as the cognitive phase, we intellectualize the task and discover new strategies to accomplish it more proficiently. During the second, the associative phase, we concentrate less, making fewer major errors, and become more efficient. Finally we reach what Fitts and Posner called the autonomous phase, when we’re as good as we need to be at the task and we basically run on autopilot. Most of the time that’s a good thing. The less we have to focus on the repetitive tasks of everyday life, the more we can concentrate on the stuff that really matters. You can actually see this phase shift take place in f.M.R.I.’s of subjects as they learn new tasks: the parts of the brain involved in conscious reasoning become less active, and other parts of the brain take over. You could call it the O.K. plateau.

Psychologists used to think that O.K. plateaus marked the upper bounds of innate ability. In his 1869 book “Hereditary Genius,” Sir Francis Galton argued that a person could improve at mental and physical activities until he hit a wall, which “he cannot by any education or exertion overpass.” In other words, the best we can do is simply the best we can do. But Ericsson and his colleagues have found over and over again that with the right kind of effort, that’s rarely the case. They believe that Galton’s wall often has much less to do with our innate limits than with what we consider an acceptable level of performance. They’ve found that top achievers typically follow the same general pattern. They develop strategies for keeping out of the autonomous stage by doing three things: focusing on their technique, staying goal-oriented and getting immediate feedback on their performance. Amateur musicians, for example, tend to spend their practice time playing music, whereas pros tend to work through tedious exercises or focus on difficult parts of pieces. Similarly, the best ice skaters spend more of their practice time trying jumps that they land less often, while lesser skaters work more on jumps they’ve already mastered. In other words, regular practice simply isn’t enough. For all of our griping over our failing memories — the misplaced keys, the forgotten name, the factoid stuck on the tip of the tongue — our biggest failing may be that we forget how rarely we forget.To improve, we have to be constantly pushing ourselves beyond where we think our limits lie and then pay attention to how and why we fail. That’s what I needed to do if I was going to improve my memory.

With typing, it’s relatively easy to get past the O.K. plateau. Psychologists have discovered that the most efficient method is to force yourself to type 10 to 20 percent faster than your comfort pace and to allow yourself to make mistakes. Only by watching yourself mistype at that faster speed can you figure out the obstacles that are slowing you down and overcome them. Ericsson suggested that I try the same thing with cards. He told me to find a metronome and to try to memorize a card every time it clicked. Once I figured out my limits, he instructed me to set the metronome 10 to 20 percent faster and keep trying at the quicker pace until I stopped making mistakes. Whenever I came across a card that was particularly troublesome, I was supposed to make a note of it and see if I could figure out why it was giving me cognitive hiccups. The technique worked, and within a couple days I was off the O.K. plateau, and my card times began falling again at a steady clip. Before long, I was committing entire decks to memory in just a few minutes."

13 hours agoekanes

I have always thought that my brain is optimized for reading. I’m just okay at math, my sense of direction is downright bad, and I’m not that physically coordinated, but I was an avid reader from an early age. I ace reading comprehension and analogies questions on standardized tests. It has never felt like a skill I intentionally developed. I have long assumed it is an innate characteristic.